DEMOGRAPHIC FACTORS AND PARENTS’ COMMUNICATIVE ACTS: THEIR IMPLICATIONS ON MODELS OF CHILD’S USE OF LANGUAGE
(1) University of Asia and the Pacific and University of the Philippines, Philippines
(*) Corresponding Author
Abstract
This case study investigated Halliday’s models of child’s use of language involving an English-Filipino bilingual boy and how he was influenced by certain demographic factors and parents’ communicative acts. Data was from nine videos that captured the child’s naturally occurring interactions involving his parents and family friends between the ages 2.6 and 4.10 within a span of almost three years. The multimethod approach was used in analyzing data, namely, qualitative frequency analysis and online interview for triangulation purposes. Five of the seven functions of language in children were demonstrated and appeared to have been influenced by ethnicity, age, gender, and parents’ communicative acts and attitude but not by bilingualism as earlier predicted. More importantly, four nascent models were exhibited, suggesting that there could be more than seven language functions in children as previously posited by Halliday. The esteem function, rescue function, corrective function, and asserting function, reflective of models of child’s use of language in Filipino and Asian contexts, were discovered and such typologies are proposed in this study. Findings have implications on bilingualism, language teaching, and language development theories.
Keywords
Full Text:
PDFReferences
Akhmetova, D. Z., Chelnokova, T. A., & Morozova, I. G. (2017). Theoretical and methodological basis of inclusive education in the researches of Russian scientists in the first quarter of 20th century (P. P. Blonsky, L. S. Vygotsky, v. P. Kaschenko, S. T. Shatsky). International Education Studies, 10(2), 74-179.
Adani, S., & Cepanec, M. (2019). Sex differences in early communication development: behavioral and neurobiological indicators of more vulnerable communication system development in boys. Croatian Medical Journal, 60(2), 141–149.
Ahearn, L. M. (2017). Living language: An introduction to linguistic anthropology (2nd ed). Malden, MA: Wiley Blackwell.
Albarran, A., & Reich, H. M. (2013). Using baby books to increase new mothers' self-efficacy and improve toddler language development. Infant and Child Development, 23(4), 374-387. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1832
Allard-Kropp, M. (2020). Languages and worldview. Open Educational Resources Collection 17. Retrieved on December 8, 2021, from: https://irl.umsl.edu/oer/17
Ansah, G. N., Anderson, J. A., Anamzoya, S. A., & Ohemeng, F. (2017). ‘Bra, Sɛn, Yɛnkↄ... that is all I know in akan’: How female migrants from rural north survive with minimum bilingualism in urban markets in Ghana. Ghana Journal of Linguistics, 6(1), 49-74. http://dx.doi.org/10.4314/gjl.v6i1.69
Barel, E., & Tzischinsky, O. (2018). Age and sex differences in verbal and visuospatial abilities. Advances in cognitive psychology, 2(14), 51–61. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0238-x
Barnett, M. A., Gustafsson, H., Deng, M., Mills-Koonce, W. R., & Cox, M. (2012). Bidirectional associations among sensitive parenting, language development, and social competence. Infant and Child Development, 21 (4), 374-393. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1750
Basit, T. N., Hughes, A., Iqbal, Z., & Cooper, J. (2014). The influence of socio- economic status and ethnicity on speech and language development. International Journal of Early Years Education, 23(1), 115–133. https://doi.org/10.1080/09669760.2014.973838
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16(4), 240-250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001
Bird, E. K., Genesee, F., & Verhoeven, L. (2016). Bilingualism in children with developmental disorders: A narrative review. Journal of Communication Disorders, 63, 1-14. doi https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016.07.003
Blom, E., Boerma, T., Bosma, E., Cornips, L., & Everaert, E. (2017). Cognitive advantages of bilingual children in different sociolinguistic contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(552), 1-12. doi: http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00552
Brophy-Herb, H. E., Stansbury, K., Bocknek, E., & Horodynski, M. A. (2012). Modeling maternal emotion-related socialization behaviors in a low- income sample: Relations with toddlers’ self-regulation. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27, 352–364. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.11.005
Byrnes, J. P., & Wasik, B. A. (2009). Language and literacy development: What educators need to know. New York: The Guilford Press.
Chang, Y. E. (2017). Pathways from mothers' early social support to children's language development at age 3. Infant and Child Development, 26(6), e2025. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2025
Dawe, C. (2014). Language governmentality in Philippine education policy. Working Papers in Educational Linguistics, 29(1), 61-77.
Devitt, A. J. (2015). Genre performances: John Swales' genre analysis and rhetorical-linguistic genre studies. Journal of English for Academic Purposes, 19, 44-51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jeap.2015.05.008.
Dimaculangan, N. G. (2018). Another look into Philippine English: Towards users’ awareness and celebration. International Journal of Advance Research and Publications, 2(8), 17-22.
Dita, S. (2009). The metalinguistic awareness of Filipino bilingual children. Philippine ESL Journal, 3, 6-24.
Etchell, A., Adhikari., A, Weinberg, L. S., Choo, A. L., Garnett, E. O., Chow, H. M., & Chang, S. (2018). A systematic literature review of sex differences in childhood language and brain development. Neuropsychologia, 114, 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.011.
Faitar, G. M. (2011). Socioeconomic status, ethnicity and the context of achievement in minority education. Journal of Instructional Pedagogies, 5, 1-8.
Halliday, M. A. K. (2002). Relevant models of language. In Power, B.M. & Hubbard, R. S. (Eds.) Language development: A reader for teachers (2nd ed.). New Jersey: Merrill Prentice Hall.
Hoft, E. (2014). Language development (5th ed.). California: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.
Kapa, L. L., & Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development, 28, 233– 246. doi http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.01.011
Kokštejn, J., Musálek, M., & Tufano, J. J. (2017). Are sex differences in fundamental motor skills uniform throughout the entire preschool period? PLoS One, 12, e0176556. doi: http://do.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0176556.
Kousaie, S., Sheppard, C., Lemieux, M., Monetta, L., & Taler, V. (2014). Executive function and bilingualism in young and older adults. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience, 8(250), 1-12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00250
Language development in children: 0-8 years (2021). Raisingchildren.net.au. Retrieved from https://raisingchildren.net.au/babies/development/language- development/language-development-0-8
Ledesma, H. M. L., & Morris, R. D. (2011). Patterns of language preference among bilingual (Filipino–English) boys. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 8(1), 62-80. https://doi.org/10.1080/jBEB.v8.i1.pg62
Lester, D. (2013). Measuring maslow’s hierarchy of needs. Psychological Reports, 113(1), 15–17. https://doi.org/10.2466/02.20.PR0.113x16z1
Lukens-Bull, R., & Zahn, K. A. (2018). The linguistic modeling of variation in islam: Constructing saussure’s concepts of langue and parole to religious studies. Walisongo: Jurnal Penelitian Sosial Keagamaan, 26(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.21580/WS.26.1.2088
Madigan, S., Prime, H., Graham, S. A., Rodrigues, M., Anderson, N., Khoury, J., & Jenkins, J. M. (2019). Parenting behavior and child language: A meta- analysis. Pediatrics, 144(4), e20183556. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2018-3556
Madigan, S., Wade, M., Plamondon, A., Browne, D., & Jenkins, J. M. (2015). Birth weight variability and language development: Risk, resilience, and responsive parenting. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 40(9), 869- 77. doi: http://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsv056
O’Grady, W. (2005). How children learn language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Pancsofar, N., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2006). Mother and father language input to young children: Contributions to later language development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(6), 571-587. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2006.08.003.
Pinquart, M., & Gerke, D. C. (2019). Associations of parenting styles with self- esteem in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Journal of Child Family Studies, 28, 2017–2035. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019- 01417-5
Ramayanti, D., & Marlina, L. (2018). The analysis of types illocutionary acts in “Tangled” movie. E-Journal of English Language & Literature, 7(1), 27- 34. https://doi.org/10.24036/ell.v7i1.8800
Stoehr, A., Benders, T., Van Hell, J., & Fikkert, P. (2018). Heritage language exposure impacts voice onset time of Dutch–German simultaneous bilingual preschoolers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(3), 598- 617. http://doi:10.1017/S1366728917000116
Tanaka, N., O’Grady, W., Deen, K., Kim, C., Hattori, R., Bondoc, I. P. M., & Soriano, J. U. (2014). Acquisition of tagalog focus system. The 16th Annual International Conference of the Japanese Society for Language Sciences.
Thwaite, A. (2019). Halliday’s view of child language learning: Has it been misinterpreted? Australian Journal of Teacher Education, 44(5), 42-56.
Toivainen, T., Papageorgiou, K. A., Tosto, M. G., & Kovas, Y. (2017). Sex differences in non-verbal and verbal abilities in childhood and adolescence. Intelligence, 64, 81-88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2017.07.007.
Tsui, R., Tong, X., & Chan, C. (2019). Impact of language dominance on phonetic transfer in Cantonese– English bilingual language switching. Applied Psycholinguistics, 40(1), 29-58. http://doi:10.1017/S0142716418000449
Tupas, R. (2015). The politics of ‘p’ and ‘f’: a linguistic history of ‘nation- building’ in the Philippines. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 36(6), 587-597. http://doi:10.1080/01434632.2014.979831
Unsworth, S. (2013). Current issues in multilingual first language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 33, 21-50. http://doi:10.1017/S0267190513000044
Van Dijk, C., Van Wonderen, E., Koutamanis, E., Kootstra, G., Dijkstra, T., & Unsworth, S. (2021). Cross-linguistic influence in simultaneous and early sequential bilingual children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Child Language, 48(6), 1-33. http://doi:10.1017/S0305000921000337
Wade, M., Jenkins, J. M., Venkadasalam, V. P., Binnoon-Erez, N., & Ganea, P. A. (2018). The role of maternal responsiveness and linguistic input in pre- academic skill development: A longitudinal analysis of pathways. Cognitive Development, 45, 125– 140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.01.005
Wattimena, R. U., & Manara, C. (2016). Language use in shifting contexts: Two multilingual Filipinos’ narratives of language and mobility. Indonesian Journal of English Language Teaching, 11(2), 153-167
DOI: https://doi.org/10.24071/ijhs.v5i2.3951
Refbacks
- There are currently no refbacks.
Copyright (c) 2022 Rafael Ibe Santos
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.
Indexed and abstracted in:
IJHS Sinta 3 Certificate (S3 = Level 3)
International Journal of Humanity Studies (IJHS) has been nationally accredited Sinta 3 by the Ministry of Education, Culture, Research and Technology of the Republic of Indonesia based on the decree No. Surat Keputusan 158/E/KPT/2021. Validity for 5 years: Vol 4 No 1, 2020 till Vol 8 No 2, 2024
This work is licensed under CC BY-SA.
Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.
p-ISSN: 2597-470X (since 31 August 2017); e-ISSN: 2597-4718 (since 31 August 2017)
Notice: The opinions expressed in this publication are those of the authors. They do not purport to reflect the opinions or views of the editorial team or publishers.
International Journal of Humanity Studies (IJHS) is a scientific journal in English published twice a year, namely in September and March, by Sanata Dharma University, Yogyakarta, Indonesia.